Submit or Track your Manuscript LOG-IN

New Experimental Observations of Infanticide and Cannibalism in Northern Tree Shrew Tupaia belangeri

PJZ_55_1_49-55

New Experimental Observations of Infanticide and Cannibalism in Northern Tree Shrew Tupaia belangeri

Dong-Min Hou, Hong-bi Peng, Wan-Long Zhu* and Zheng-Kun Wang*

Key Laboratory of Ecological Adaptive Evolution and Conservation on Animals-Plants in Southwest Mountain Ecosystem of Yunnan, School of Life Sciences, Yunnan Normal University, Kunming, Yunnan 650500, China.

ABSTRACT

Maternal infanticide and cannibalism, wherein mothers kill and consume their offspring, commonly occur in various animal taxa. The extent of its occurrence in Tupaia belangeri remains unclear given the difficulty of observing infanticidal and cannibalistic behaviors. Here, we report observations of maternal infanticide and cannibalism in two female T. belangeri in two laboratory experiments. Our footage provides the first video documentation of infanticide, cannibalism, and parental care behavior in T. belangeri. Analysis of the video data indicated that the observations were related to nutrition hypothesis, hormone level hypothesis and stress hypothesis. We also suggest that the infanticidal and cannibalistic behaviors of tree shrews might be related to their dietary habits. Although reactions to infants are doubtless affected by the psychological and physiological status of tree shrew mothers as well as the environment, additional work is needed to clarify the relative roles of various factors in shaping maternal behavior post-parturition.


Article Information

Received 20 August 2021

Revised 03 September 2021

Accepted 21 September 2021

Available online 08 February 2022

(early access)

Published 19 October 2022

Authors’ Contribution

D-MH and H-BP carried out the experiment. W-LZ and Z-KW conceived of the study and participated in its design, coordination and drafted the manuscript.

Key words

Tupaia belangeri, Infanticide, Cannibalism, Nutrition, Hormone level, Stress, Dietary habits

DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.17582/journal.pjz/20210820090809

* Corresponding author: zwl_8307@163.com, wangzk_123@163.com

0030-9923/2023/0001-49 $ 9.00/0

Copyright 2023 by the authors. Licensee Zoological Society of Pakistan.

This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).



Introduction

Infanticide and cannibalism are widespread in the animal kingdom. Several recent studies of infanticide and cannibalism have been conducted in various animals, including primates (Yao et al., 2016), rodents (Schmidt et al., 2015), ungulates (Gray, 2009) and birds (Harris et al., 2016). Most mammals have an instinctive response to protect and nurture their own offspring, as such behavior is favored by natural selection (Rilling and Young, 2014). However, infanticide and cannibalism are the antithesis of offspring care (Hrdy, 2016). Several explanations have been proposed to explain the occurrence of infanticide (Table I) (Kral et al., 2019). Cannibalism is the consumption of all or part of another conspecific (Richardson et al., 2010). In mammals, cannibalism often occurs following infanticide (Culot et al., 2011). Cannibalism of offspring may occur to aid resource acquisition when resources are limited (Bronson and Marsteller, 1985; Hrdy, 1979). In rare cases, infanticide leads to the cannibalization of dead juveniles (Fox, 1975; Hrdy, 1979). The incidence of cannibalism may be affected by the metabolic requirements of the adults, which can vary with sex, age, and reproductive state (Ebensperger et al., 2000).

Tupaia belangeri (Tupaiidae: Scandentia) is the only member of Tupaiidae in China. As its metabolic system and anatomical structure are more similar to humans than to mice, T. belangeri has been widely used as an experimental animal model in biomedical research (Zhu et al., 2010; Hou et al., 2021). It occurs in tropical rain forest, secondary forest, and bush habitat and feeds on insects and fruit (Hou et al., 2018a). Larvae of the genus Tenebrio are their preferred food, and fruit is their second choice (Zhang et al., 2017; Peng et al., 2020). Although plant food resources are abundant, animal food resources in their natural habitat are scarce by comparison. They have a strong reproductive capacity, and the number of fetuses generally ranges from 2 to 5 (Yao, 2017).

Here, we provide new behavioral observations of parental infanticide and cannibalism in T. belangeri from two different laboratory experiments. The aim of this study was to explore the causes of infanticide and cannibalism in T. belangeri and provide insights that could inspire future breeding experiments.

Materials and methods

Observation of individual behavior

This study involves two experiments. In the first experiment the individual behavior was observed. T. belangeri were captured at the boscage of Luquan County, Yunnan Province in the spring of 2017 and maintained at the School of Life Sciences, Yunnan Normal University, Kunming, China. The animals (n=6, ♀3: ♂3) were acclimated to ambient temperature 25±1°C, kept under a 12L:12D light/dark cycle, and were provided water ad libitum for whole process of the experiment. Individual behavior (activity behavior, resting behavior, feeding behavior, modifying behavior) was observed for 28 d. Animals were provided with adequate larvae of Tenebrio molitor and apples during the pre-parturition period. As it was clear that only one T. belangeri were pregnant before the experiment, observations of the pre-parturition and post-parturition behavior in T. belangeri were made; the physiological state of the infants was also determined. The purpose of experiments 1 was to observe the behavior of tree shrews, and see (Hou et al., 2018b; c; 2019) for relevant experimental results.

Effects of light on behavior, memory and oxidative stress

The effects of light intensity on the behavior, learning memory, and oxidative stress of Tupaia belangeri was observed in the second experiment. T. belangeri were captured at the boscage of Luquan County, Yunnan Province in the summer of 2019 and maintained at the School of Life Sciences, Yunnan Normal University, Kunming, China. The animals (n=6 of each group, ♀6: ♂12) were acclimated to ambient temperature 25±1°C, kept under a 12L: 12D light/dark cycle, and were provided water ad libitum for whole process of the experiment. The animals were randomly divided into three groups: 50-W illumination group, 100-W illumination group, and 200-W illumination group. The appropriate/natural brightness under general rearing conditions of tree shrews was 15-W. During the experiment, the animals were placed in behavioral arena, and an incandescent lamp was suspended at 120 cm above the device for light stimulation. Experiments were run from 9:00 to 17:00 daily and individual behavior (activity behavior, resting behavior, feeding behavior, modifying behavior) was observed for 28 d. Animals were fed daily from 7:00 to 8:00 and from 18:00 to 19:00. Tree shrews showing infanticide and cannibalism behavior were in the 200-W illumination group; however, the experimental animals in this group did not appear to be pregnant prior to the start of the experiment. But over time, one animal was found to be pregnant. The purpose of experiments 2 was to investigate the effects of different light intensities on the behavior changes, learning memory ability and oxidative stress in T. belangeri, and see (Peng et al., 2021) for relevant experimental results.

 

Table I. List of hypotheses explaining the occurrence of infanticide and cannibalism in mammals.

Hypothesis

Definition and description

References

Exploitation

The infanticidal animal benefits from consumption or use of the victim.

Agrell et al., 1998; Hrdy, 1979

Resource competition

The infanticidal animal competes with other adult individuals for physical resources such as food or nest sites.

Agrell et al., 1998; Hrdy, 1979; Manning et al., 1995

Food scarcity

Cannibalism may be an interaction that reduces population size before acute resource shortage causes severe physiological stress.

Fox, 1975

Compensate of pregnancy

Cannibalistic behaviour after a short period of maternal care is that it allows mothers to compensate for the costs of pregnancy.

Elgar and Crespi, 1992

Paternal manipulation

By killing the infant, parents improve chances for their own survival or the survival of their existing offspring, or they may gain greater net reproductive fitness in the future. In addition, parents may manipulate the offspring sex ratio via infanticide.

Hrdy, 1979; Ebensperger, 1998

Nutrition

The nutritional hypothesis for cannibalism posits that the act of consuming a conspecific provides participating individuals with nutritional benefits that might improve future reproductive success.

Fox, 1975; Klug and Bonsall, 2007

Adoption avoidance

An individual commits infanticide to avoid adopting and providing parental care, especially allosuckling, to unrelated offspring.

Ebensperger, 1998

Sexual selection

Infant killing is directed at offspring unlikely to be descendants of the killer, so reducing the reproductive success of competitors and increasing the nfanticidal individual’s own opportunities to breed.

Agrell et al., 1998; Hrdy, 1979; Palombit, 2015

Non-adaptive behaviour

Infanticide is a mistake or a result of stress, frustration, or even a consequence of aggression to third parties or self-destruction, etc.

Hrdy, 1979; Cassini, 1998; Palombit, 2015

Accidental behaviour

This would seem more likely to be an attempt to remove the newborn rather than a deliberate attempt to kill and eat the neonate.

Singh and Garcia, 2015; Smith and Smith, 2019

 

Note: Some of the descriptions of hypotheses are quoted from Kral et al., 2019.

 

The experimental procedures were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the School of Life Sciences, Yunnan Normal University (No. 13-0901-011).

Behavioral observation

For behavioral observation the behavioral arena is a special instrument made of a transparent acrylic plate, and a real-time surveillance camera was located at the top of the behavioral arena. Animals were also provided with a box for feeding in the device. The animal houses was equipped with a 15-W incandescent lamp. After each experiment, 75% alcohol was used to wipe and disinfect the device.

Data anlysis

The video data obtained by the real-time surveillance camera were stored on a hard disk. Because the hard disks were typically full after 7 d, the data were extracted from the hard disks every 7 d. Video software was used to analyze tree shrew behavior.

Results

Pre-parturition and post-parturition behavior

Prior to delivery in experiment 1, tree shrews did not engage in strenuous activities, spent most of their time licking their abdomen and pudenda, and occasionally fed. Births occurred rapidly during the delivery period, and body movements were infrequent. Consequently, it was impossible to determine the specific birth order and birth time of the five infants. Following delivery, adults spent approximately 18 min licking the head and body parts of the infants and 5 min licking their pudenda and other body parts. During this period, we observed tree shrews cover the infants with small pieces of paper is shown in Video 1. Thereafter, the animals frequently fed.

In experiment 2, tree shrews often engaged in strenuous activities, and they did not lick their pudenda prior to delivery. Tree shrews exhibited various body movements during the delivery period. The birth order and birth time of the three infants were noted. The tree shrews licked No. 2-1 for only approximately 3s during the entire delivery period. Following delivery, tree shrews were highly active and frequently fed.

Infanticide and cannibalism information

In experiment 1, no infanticide was observed, and all five infants died of natural causes (Table II). However, cannibalism was only observed for No.1-3 the day after delivery. The entire body of the infant was consumed at one time, and the consumption of this individual took approximately 740s.

In experiment 2, infanticide and cannibalism were observed for three infants 66s after delivery (Table II). Cannibalism occurred immediately after infanticide. It took approximately 185s for the entire body of No.2-1 to be consumed. It took 221 s for the entire body of No.2-2 to be consumed, 135 s for all of the body parts except the head, and 86 s for the head, and the body parts were consumed before the head. It took approximately 133 s for the entire body of No. 2-3 to be consumed, 101 s for the head, and 32 s for the remaining body parts, and the head was consumed before the body parts. See Video 2 for details of cannibalism in T. belangeri.

Discussion

The metabolic levels of hormones such as oxytocin and steroids are especially high during the second half of pregnancy and can affect maternal behavior (Saltzman and Maestripieri, 2011; Bercovitch, 2020). Animals can go from treating an infant cadaver as alive to treating it as food, and these behavioral changes might be mediated by physiological changes in hormone levels (Watson and Matsuzawa, 2018). T. belangeri covered the infants with small pieces of paper, which resembled the way that human mothers often show love for their children. However, some T. belangeri mothers also killed and consumed their young. The occurrence of cannibalism at the end of pregnancy might thus reflect a gradual weakening of the mother-infant relationship and a change in hormone metabolism (Trapanese et al., 2020). Elgar and Crespi (1992) suggested that the occurrence of cannibalism following a short period of maternal care might be driven by mothers compensating for the energy consumed during pregnancy. Our observations support this hypothesis in experiment 1: the tree shrews took care of their offspring post-parturition, and cannibalism occurred long after parturition (Table II). Thus, the first explanation for cannibalism might be that the hormone levels of the tree shrews changed during pregnancy.

T. belangeri are omnivorous and prefer to consume T. molitor larvae (Peng et al., 1991). Sufficient food was provided in the early stage of experiment 1 and 2. However, there was a shortage of animal food resources (T. molitor larvae) in the later stage of the experiment, plant food resources (apple) was still abundant. Food scarcity is considered one of the main factors driving infanticide and cannibalism in animals (Getto et al., 2005; Duarte et al., 2010). Cannibalism has often been observed to occur when food resources are abundant, which contradicts ecological theory predicting that cannibalism mostly occurs where resources are scarce (Meek and Brown, 2017). However, these observations are consistent with the nutrition hypothesis (Hrdy, 1979; Ebensperger, 1998), which predicts that cannibalism might be selectively favored because consuming conspecifics provides nutritional benefits that may increase future fitness (Klug and Bonsall, 2007; Nishikawa et al., 2020). This hypothesis may also explain the observations in our study. Meat was not provided as food late in experiment 1 and 2. Thus, tree shrews might have been motivated by the desire to consume meat. Generally, the need for carnivores to consume meat might lead to cannibalism. We suggest that the infanticide and cannibalism of tree shrews might be related to their dietary habits, namely their preference for consuming Tenebrio molitor larvae.

According to Fox (1975), physiological or psychological stress has been associated with infanticide and cannibalism. Maternal cannibalism has also been suggested to be induced by unnatural laboratory environments and thus represents a non-adaptive behavior (Fedurek et al., 2020). Stressful conditions can lead to a higher incidence of infanticide (Rimbach et al., 2012). The biological responses of rodents may be affected by abnormal environments, such as temperature, photoperiod, and noise (Besch, 1980).

 

Table II. Notes on infanticide and cannibalismin Tupaia belangerifrom two laboratory experiments.

Information

Experiment 1

Experiment 2

Original goal

Observation on individual behavior of T. belangeri

Effects of light intensity on behavior, learning memory and oxidative stress of Tupaia belangeri

Date

2017.4.10-2017.5.8

2019.8.22-2019.9.19

Type of feed

Larvae of Tenebrio molitor and apple

Standard solid feed

Experimental condition

Normal environment

200W illumination

Start time of parturition

4.16 09:59:20

8.25 10:33:48

End time of parturition

4.16 10:05:23

8.25 10:35:51

Duration of parturition (s)

363

123

Time ranges of lick behavior

4.16 10:09:53-10:38:28

8.25 10:34:12-10:34:15

Birth time in infants

The scope of birth sites was relatively concentrated, it was impossible to observe the birth time of each infants

8.25 10:33:51 No.2-1

8.25 10:34:18 No.2-2

8.25 10:35:51 No.2-3

Litter size

5

3

Number of infanticides

0

3

Number of cannibalism

1

3

Death time in infants

4.16 19:00:52 No.1-1

8.25 10:36:57 No.2-1

4.16 22:28:37 No.1-2

8.25 10:46:01 No.2-2

4.17 04:22:48 No.1-3

8.25 14:03:02 No.2-3

4.17 08:34:24 No.1-4

4.17 10:56:06 No.1-5

Manner of death in infants

Natural death

Be killed by mother

Time ranges of cannibalism

4.17 14:15:02-14:27:22

Cannibalised of No.1-3

8.25 10:36:57-10:40:02

Cannibalised of No.2-1

8.25 10:46:01-10:48:16

Cannibalised of No.2-2

8.25 11:59:09-12:00:35

Cannibalised the remnant of No.2-2

8.25 14:03:02-14:04:43

Cannibalised of No.2-3

8.25 16:34:47-16:35:19

Cannibalised the remnant of No.2-3

 

Note: Because it was impossible to observe the birth time of each infant, they were referred to as No.1-X and No.2-X in accordance with their death time in experiment 1 and experiment 2, respectively.

 

Tree shrews were clearly restless under the high strength of illumination in experiment 2; this, coupled with the fact that they killed their offspring and consumed them shortly after delivery, is consistent with the non-adaptive behavior hypothesis. The third possible explanation for the infanticidal and cannibalistic behaviors is that they reflect a response to psychological stress under abnormal environments (200-W illumination).

In summary, our study provides the first analysis of infanticide and cannibalism in T. belangeri as well as the first evidence of nursing and caring behaviors. Nutrition hypothesis, hormone level hypothesis and stress hypothesis are all related to the observations of cannibalism, which we expect is might related to the dietary habits of T. belangeri. In addition, because of the limited number of experimental animal samples, we need to further expand the number of samples to further determine which hypothesis led to this phenomenon.

Acknowledgments

The present study was financially supported by the National Science Foundation of China (No. 31760118), Young and Middle-aged Academic and Technical Leaders Reserve Talents Project of Yunnan Province (2019HB013), and Young Top-Notch Talent for “Ten Thousand Talents Program” of Yunnan (YNWR-QNRC-2019-047). We thank Liwen Bianji, Edanz Editing China (www.liwenbianji.cn/ac), for editing the English text of a draft of this manuscript.

Supplementary material

There is supplementary material associated with this article. Access the material online at: https://dx.doi.org/10.17582/journal.pjz/20210820090809

Statement of conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

Agrell, J., Wolff, J.O. and Ylönen, H. 1998. Counter-strategies to infanticide in mammals: Costs and consequences. Oikos, 83: 507-517. https://doi.org/10.2307/3546678

Besch, E.L., 1980. Environmental quality within animal facilities. Lab. Anim. Sci., 30: 385-406.

Bercovitch, F.B., 2020. A comparative perspective on the evolution of mammalian reactions to dead conspecifics. Primates, 61: 21-28. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00722-3

Bronson, F.H. and Marsteller, F.A., 1985. Effect of short-term food deprivation on reproduction in female mice. Biol. Reprod., 33: 660-667. https://doi.org/10.1095/biolreprod33.3.660

Cassini, M.H., 1998. Inter-specific infanticide in South American otariids. Behaviour, 35: 1005-1012. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853998792913456

Culot, L., Lledo-Ferrer, Y., Hoelscher, O., Lazo, F.J.M., Huynen. M.C. and Heymann, E.W., 2011. Reproductive failure, possible maternal infanticide, and cannibalism in wild moustached tamarins, Saguinus mystax. Primates, 52: 179-186. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-011-0238-6

Duarte, C., Jaramillo, E., Contreras, H. and Acuña, K., 2010. Cannibalisn and food availability in the talitrid amphipod Orchestoidea tuberculata. J. Sea Res., 64: 417-421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seares.2010.02.009

Ebensperger, L.A., 1998. Strategies and counterstrategies to infanticide in mammals. Biol. Rev., 73: 321-346. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0006323198005209

Ebensperger, L.A., Botto-Mahan, C. and Tamarin, R.H., 2000. Nonparental infanticide in meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus: The influence of nutritional benefits. Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 12: 149-160. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927014.2000.9522810

Elgar, M.A. and Crespi, B.J., 1992. Cannibalism: Ecology and evolution among diverse taxa. Oxford University Press, Oxford (England), New York.

Fedurek, P., Tkaczynski, P., Asiimwe, C., Hobaiter, C., Samuni, L., Lowe, A.E., Dijrian, A.G., Zuberbuhler, K., Wittig, R.M. and Crockford, C., 2020. Maternal cannibalism in two populations of wild chimpanzees. Primates, 61: 181-187. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00765-6

Fox, L.R., 1975. Cannibalism in natural populations. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 6: 87-106. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.06.110175.000511

Getto, P., Diekman, O. and de Roos, A.M., 2005. . On the (dis) advantages of cannibalism. J. Math Biol., 51: 695-712. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00285-005-0342-6

Gray, M.E., 2009. An infanticide attempt by a free-roaming feral stallion (Equus caballus). Biol. Lett., 5: 23-25. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0571

Harris, S.N., Wagner, L.M., Ragheb, E.L.H. and Miller, K.E., 2016. Infanticide in a Florida grasshopper sparrow (Ammodramus savannarum floridanus). Wilson J. Ornithol., 128: 918-920. https://doi.org/10.1676/15-204.1

Hou, D.M., Jia, T., Li, Q., Wang, Z.K. and Zhu, W.L., 2021. Metabonomics of white adipose tissue and brown adipose tissue in Tupaia belangeri during cold acclimation. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part D Genom. Proteom., 38: 100823. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbd.2021.100823

Hou, D.M., Wang, Z.K., Ye, F.Y. and Zhu, W.L., 2018a. Male Tupaia belangeri distinguish reproductive stage of females based on urine odor. China J. Wildl., 39: 92-98.

Hou, D.M., Ren, X.Y., Wang, Z.K., Ye, F.Y. and Zhu, W.L., 2018b. Observational research of social behavior in Tupaia belangeri. J. Green Sci. Technol., 2: 4-6.

Hou, D.M., Wang, Z.K., Zhang, H. and Zhu, W.L., 2018c. Observational research of kindred behavior in Tupaia belangeri. J. Green Sci. Technol., 12: 1-5.

Hou, D.M., Wang, Z.K. and Zhu, W.L., 2019. Study on the kin recognition of Tupaia belangeri based on behavior and odor. J. Biol., 36: 50-54.

Hrdy, S.B., 1979. Infanticide among animals: a review, classification and examination of the implications for the reproductive stratigies of females. Ethol. Sociobiol., 1: 13-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(79)90004-9

Hrdy, S.B., 2016. Variable postpartum responsiveness among humans and other primates with “cooperative breeding”: A comparative and evolutionary perspective. Horm. Behav., 77: 272-283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.016

Klug, H. and Bonsall, M.B., 2007. When to care for, abandon, or eat your offspring: the evolution of parental care and filial cannibalism. Am. Nat., 170: 886-901. https://doi.org/10.1086/522936

Kral, P., Bukovsky, J., Pluhacek, J., Voldrichova, M. and Robovsky, J., 2019. A case of female-perpetrated infanticide in captive white-lipped deer (Cervus albirostris). J. Ethol., 37: 377-382. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-019-00603-8

Manning, C.J., Dewsbury, D.A., Wakeland, E.K. and Potts, W.K., 1995. Communal nesting and communal nursing in house mice, Mus musculus domesticus. Anim. Behav., 50: 741-751. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(95)80134-0

Meek, P.D. and Brown, S.C., 2017. It’s a dog eat dog world: observations of dingo (Canis familiaris) cannibalism. Aust. Mammal, 39: 92-94. https://doi.org/10.1071/AM16018

Nishikawa, M., Ferrero, N., Cheves, S., Lopez, R., Kawamura, S., Fedigan, L.M., Melin, A.D. and Jack, K.M., 2020. Infant cannibalism in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys. Ecol. Evol., 10: 12679-12684. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.6901

Palombit, R.A., 2015. Infanticide as sexual conflict: coevolution of male strategies and female counterstrategies. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol., 7: a017640. https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a017640

Peng, H.B., Hou, D.M., Zhang, D. and Zhu, W.L., 2020. Effects of food restriction on body mass, energy metabolism and thermogenesis in a tree shrew (Tupaia belangeri). Anim. Biol., 70: 175-187.

Peng, H.B., J, T., Wang, Z.K. and Zhu, W.L., 2021. Effects of light intensity on behavior, learning memory and oxidative stress of Tupaia belangeri. J. Biol., https://kns.cnki.net/kcms/detail/34.1081.Q.20210521.1128.014.html

Peng, Y.Z., Ye, Z.Z., Zou, R.J., Wang, Y.X., Tian, B.P., Ma, Y.Y. and Shi, L.M. 1991. Biology of Chinese tree shrews. Science and Technology Press, Kunming.

Richardson, M.L., Mitchell, R.F., Reagel, P.F. and Hanks, L.M., 2010. Causes and consequences of cannibalism in noncarnivorous insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol., 55: 39-53. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ento-112408-085314

Rillin, J.K. and Young, L.J., 2014. The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. Science, 345: 771-776. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1252723

Rimbach, R., Pardo-Martinez, A., Montes-Rojas, A., Di Fiore, A. and Link, A., 2012. Interspecific infanticide and infant-directed aggression by spider monkeys (Ateles hybridus) in a fragmented forest in Colombia. Am. J. Primatol., 74: 990-997. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22052

Saltzman, W. and Maestripieri, D., 2011. The neuroendocrinology of primate maternal behavior. Psychiatry, 35: 1192-1204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.09.017

Schmidt, J., Kosztolanyi, A., Tokolyi, J., Hugyecz, B., Illes, I., Kiraly, R. and Barta, Z., 2015. Reproductive asynchrony and infanticide in house mice breeding communally. Anim. Behav., 101: 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.12.015

Singh, M.D. and Garcia, G.W., 2015. Perimortality in a captive reared agouti (Dasyprocta leporina). Wildl. Biol. Pract., 11: 70-74.

Smith, R.L. and Smith, P., 2019. Wild observation of infanticide and cannibalism by Azara’s Agouti (Dasyptocta azarae) (Mammalia: Dasyproctidae) in Paraguay. Ethology, 125: 846-850. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12937

Trapanese, C., Bey, M., Tonachella, G., Meunier, H. and Masi, S., 2020. Prolonged care and cannibalism of infant corpse by relatives in semi-free-ranging capuchin monkeys. Primates, 61: 41-47. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00747-8

Watson, C.F.I. and Matsuzawa, T., 2018. Behaviour of nonhuman primate mothers toward their dead infants: uncovering mechanisms. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 373: 20170261. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0261

Yao, H., Yu, H.L., Yang, B.H., Yang, W.J., Xu, H.Q., Grueter, C., Li, M. and Xiang, Z.F., 2016. Male infanticide in the golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana), a seasonally breeding primate. Int. J. Primatol., 37: 175-184. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9892-2

Yao, Y.G., 2017. Creating animal models, why not use the Chinese tree shrew (Tupaia belangeri chinensis)? Zool. Res., 38: 118-126. https://doi.org/10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2017.032

Zhang, L., Yang, F., Wang, Z.K. and Zhu, W.L., 2017. Role of thermal physiology and bioenergetics on adaptation in tree shrew (Tupaia belangeri): the experiment test. Sci. Rep., 7: 41352. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-07317-2

Zhu, W.L., Xie, J., Lian, X. and Wang, Z.K., 2010. Thermogenic characteristics and evaporative water loss in the tree shrew (Tupaia belangeri). J. Therm. Biol., 35: 290-294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtherbio.2010.06.005

To share on other social networks, click on any share button. What are these?

Pakistan Journal of Zoology

October

Pakistan J. Zool., Vol. 56, Iss. 5, pp. 2001-2500

Featuring

Click here for more

Subscribe Today

Receive free updates on new articles, opportunities and benefits


Subscribe Unsubscribe