Submit or Track your Manuscript LOG-IN

The Dominance Hierarchy of the Female Yunnan Snub-Nosed Monkeys, Rhinopithecus bieti

PJZ_53_5_1881-1887

The Dominance Hierarchy of the Female Yunnan Snub-Nosed Monkeys, Rhinopithecus bieti

Kai Huang1,2, Wancai Xia1,2, Yi Fu3, Yaqiong Wan4, Hao Feng2, Ali Krzton5, Jiaqi Li4,* and Dayong Li1,2,*

1Institute of Rare Animals and Plants, China West Normal University, Nanchong 637009, Sichuan Province, China

2Key Laboratory of Southwest China Wildlife Resources Conservation (Ministry of Education), China West Normal University, Nanchong 637009, Sichuan Province, China

3Irradiation Preservation Key Laboratory of Sichuan Province, Sichuan Institute of Atomic Energy, Chengdu 610101, Sichuan Province, China

4Environmental Protection Key Laboratory on Biosafety, Nanjing Institute of Environmental Science, Ministry of Ecology and Environment, Nanjing 210042, Jiangsu Province, China

5RBD Library, Auburn University, Auburn 36849, Alabama, USA

Kai Huang and Wancai Xia have contributed equally to this work.

ABSTRACT

Dominance hierarchies are common in social mammals, especially primates. The formation of social hierarchies is conducive to solving the problem of the allocation of scarce resources among individuals. From August 2015 to July 2016, we observed a wild, provisioned Yunnan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus bieti) group at Xiangguqing in Baimaxueshan National Nature Reserve, Yunnan Province, China. Aggressive and submissive behaviors were used to investigate dominance hierarchies between female individuals in the same one-male unit (OMU), and the grooming reciprocity index was used to detect reciprocal relationships between these females within the OMU. The results showed that loose social hierarchies exist among the females in each OMU, and more dominant individuals have higher grooming incomes. These results are consistent with the aggressive-submissive hypothesis and the resource control hypothesis.


Article Information

Received 21 June 2020

Revised 29 July 2020

Accepted 20 August 2020

Available online 30 July 2021

Authors’ Contribution

WX, KH, JL and DL conceived the ideas and methodology. WX, YF, HF, YW and KH collected the data. WX and KH analyzed the data. AK and DL revised the manuscript and contributed to the development of ideas.

Key words

Dominance hierarchy, Rhinopithecus bieti, Aggressive-submissive hypothesis, Resource control hypothesis.

DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.17582/journal.pjz/20200621150639

* Corresponding authors: [email protected];

[email protected]

0030-9923/2021/0005-1881 $ 9.00/0

Copyright 2021 Zoological Society of Pakistan



Introduction

Dominance hierarchies are one outcome of the long-term evolution of social animals (Noë et al., 1980). Individuals may form dominance hierarchies in order to stabilize the community structure, optimize the use of natural resources, avoid casualties from fights between individuals, and protect weaker group members so as to better adapt to the ecological conditions they experience (Campbell et al., 2006). A dominance hierarchy refers to a ranked order of individuals within a social group as reflected by certain behavioral patterns. Dominance hierarchies can allow certain individuals to control desirable resources and maintain unit stability (Robert, 2005). Within the primate order, dominance hierarchies have been significant adaptations to species’ gregarious lifestyles (Walters and Seyfarth, 1987). Hierarchy is common in social mammals, especially the primates (Melnick et al., 1987). Recently, primate dominance hierarchies have become a hot topic in behavioral ecology, including studies of Rhinopithecus roxellana, Macaca thibetana, M. fuscata, M. assamensis, Pan paniscus, Papio hamadryas and Saimiri sciureus (Zhang et al., 2008). On this basis, some researchers proposed the aggressive-submissive hypothesis and the priority occupy resources hypothesis to explain the biological function and ecological significance of the reproductive unit dominance hierarchy of primate (de Waal and Luttrell, 1989).

The aggressive-submissive hypothesis states that the dominance hierarchy is formed according to the relative superiority of senders and receivers of inter-individual aggressive and submissive behaviors (Li et al., 2006). The dominance hierarchy within a group develops after repeated competition between individuals and can be described according to the asymmetric aggression and submission patterns that occur (de Waal and Luttrell, 1989). Wang et al. (2007) studied the relationship between dominance hierarchies and food competition in Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana), and the result of higher-hierarchies individuals taking priority in food resources was consistent with this hypothesis. When environmental resources are insufficient to satisfy all individuals in the group, competition will occur (Cui et al., 2014). Over the long term, repeated bouts of aggression and submission promote stable dominance hierarchies, making it easier for higher-ranking individuals to obtain more and better resources (Wrangham, 1981). The aggressive-submissive hypothesis has empirical support in some primate species.

The resource control hypothesis suggests that higher-ranking individuals can secure better access to limited and desirable resources. Research on dominance hierarchies in R. roxellana and Cebus capucinus have shown that higher-ranking group members claim safer spaces and better food before others (Zhang et al., 2008; Desrochers, 1989). Dominance hierarchies can substantially affect the quality of life and reproductive success of female primates (Leinfelder et al., 2001). The female dominance hierarchy within a group, as reflected in the frequency and intensity of social interactions among individuals, can determine access to high-quality resources and mates (Emlen, 1982a). In polygynous primates with female dominance hierarchies, high-ranking females are more likely to be favored by unit males, and they may also restrict mating opportunities for lower-ranking females (Emlen, 1982b, 1994; Vehrencamp, 1983; Reeve and Keller, 1995). However, most primate species’ female dominance hierarchies are flexible enough that dominant females do not completely monopolize high-quality resources, allowing more submissive individuals to “purchase” the tolerance of dominant individuals through allogrooming.

Yunnan snub-nosed monkey forms large, multilevel social groups consisting of many one-male units (OMUs) and associated all-male units (AMUs) (Li et al., 2014). So far, there are no published studies of female dominance hierarchies in wild populations of R. bieti (Zhang et al., 2014), but studying such hierarchies within the one-male units (OMUs) that comprise R. bieti’s multilevel society will improve our understanding of community structure and stability in this species (Li et al., 2010, 2013). This is conducive to the protection and scientific management of this rare and endangered species (Xia et al., 2019). In this study, reliable individual identification of monkeys allowed the female dominance hierarchies within the study group to be described on the basis of bouts of aggressive and submissive behavior. Exploring the relationship between female dominance hierarchies and grooming income permitted the testing of the aggressive-submissive hypothesis and the resource control hypothesis.

Materials and methods

Study site and the study group

The study was carried out at Xiangguqing in Baimaxueshan Nature Reserve (27°36′ N, 99°15′ E, elevation: 2,400-4,200 m above sea level), Yunnan Province, China (Xia et al., 2020). The study group consisted of 8 one male units (OMUs) and one all-male unit (AMU), and the study group consisted of 64 individuals. R. bieti were individually identifiable using physical characteristics such as body size, hair pattern, scars, facial features, pelage color, and other distinctive physical features. Every day at 9:00 am and 17:00 pm, reserve staff offered food to monkeys including lichens, carrots, apples, peanuts and lacquer tree fruit. In this way, R. bieti have adapted to mild human disturbances, which creates conditions for us to observe them closely.

Behavioral definitions and data collection

The frequencies of aggressive and submissive behavior on the part of individual females were used to map dominance hierarchies within each one-male unit. According to Li et al. (2006) ethology, the following aggressive behaviors are characteristic of R. bieti: bite, snatch, catch, threat, and displacement; submissive behaviors were divided into avoidance, crouch, and escape. From August 2015 to July 2016, the effective observation time was 231 days. Aggressive and submissive behavior was observed and recorded for named individuals using focal animal sampling and all-occurrence sampling (Martin and Bateson, 1993). We take HD DGZ HL LB XS as the target observation OMU, because the field environment is complex, it is difficult to find the target observation OMU. Once found, we target the adult females in the family and keep recording their behaviors until they disappear from our field of vision. For each occurrence we recorded the initiator, the recipient, the type of conflict behavior, and the cause and effect of the conflict (Li et al., 2006).

Data analyses

We calculated values for DS and NDS to determine the dominance hierarchy of females within an OMU (de Vries et al., 2006; Gammell et al., 2003). The grooming reciprocity index was used to measure the degree of reciprocity in each female dyad in an OMU (Nishida, 1988).

R = (Gr-Ga) / (Ga+Gr)

Where, R represents the grooming reciprocity index, Ga represents the grooming time initiated by individual A, and Gr represents the grooming time received by individual A. Smaller values of R signify less benefits from grooming.

The dominance hierarchy is the absolute value of the slope of the curve plotted with values of DS on the x-axis and NDS on the y-axis and always falls between 0 and 1. A dominance hierarchy value close to 1 indicates a group that is rigidly stratified, whereas a group with a value closer to 0 is more egalitarian. Linear regression and the Spearman correlation were calculated to test the relationship between the female dominance hierarchies and grooming benefits. All collected data were processed and analyzed using Microsoft Excel 2013 and SPSS 22.0, with significance levels set to 0.05.

Results

Female dominance hierarchy within each OMU

Aggressive and submissive behavioral data was recorded for five OMUs; three other OMUs (Duanshou, Danba, and Lianheguo) were excluded on the basis of having only one female during the study period. A total of 2,947 aggressive or submissive behaviors were observed, of which 2,545 occurred between adult females within the OMU. The remaining 402 behaviors, which occurred between members of different OMUs, were not analyzed. The number of aggressive or submissive behaviors recorded for each focal OMU were as follows: HD - 873, DGZ - 588, HL - 617, LB - 402, and XS - 449 (Table I). The results showed that each OMU had a stable female dominance hierarchy: XH> ML> LY> CF> LX> ED for the HD OMU, DYB> XM> BM for the DGZ OMU, ED> BB> LJ> BM for the HL OMU, BYD> LK> CF for the LB OMU, and BM> LK> EY> EB for the XS OMU. On the other hand, these dominance hierarchies were not strict, with values closer to 0 than to 1 (Ks (HD) = 0.29; Ks(DGZ)=0.23; Ks(HL)=0.21; Ks(LB)=0.17; Ks(XS)=0.41) (Fig. 1).

Dominance hierarchy and grooming reciprocity

A total of 3,788.92 minutes of grooming was recorded between females during the study period (by OMU: HD -1272.22 min, DGZ -665.3 min, HL -881.18 min, LB -574.57 min, XS -325.65 min). Since some females migrated into or out of the focal OMUs during the study period, grooming time was normalized using the ratio (time initiating or receiving grooming/time of individual observation). The analysis showed that higher-ranking females had higher grooming incomes, with each OMU having a significantly positive Spearman correlation between female rank and grooming income (Spearman correlation test: RHD =0.886, P<0.05; RDGZ =0.999, P<0.05; RHL =0.981, P<0.05; RLB =0.998, P<0.05; RXS =0.98, P<0.05) (Fig. 2).


 

Table I.- The female dominance hierarchies among the five focal OMUs.

The data in this table respect the aggressive-submissive behavior ratio of adult females in the target unit (the horizontal axis represents the winning percentage and the vertical axis represents the losing percentage) is located in the area indicated by the dashed line in the table; the bold is the migration female during the study period.


 

Discussion

Dominance hierarchy and the aggressive-submissive hypothesis

Adult female R. bieti rarely fight each other. Yu et al. (2009) have shown that in the congeneric Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (R. roxellana), fighting behavior only occurred between males at Shennongjia (Yu et al., 2009). Similar to Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas), R. bieti is a relatively tolerant species, with hierarchies that are correspondingly loose (Coelho et al., 1983; Cui et al., 2014). Moreover, high-ranking individuals do not monopolize quality resources, giving individuals of lower rank the opportunity to obtain a share (Ren et al., 1991). Females deal with competition through ritualized behavior (i.e., Theropithecus gelada, Claud, 1970), which can determine the dominance hierarchy without the need for direct conflict and allow individuals to avoid injury (Claud, 1970; Li et al., 2006). At the same time, reproductive costs are much higher for females than males, and their reproductive success is tightly constrained by nutritional stress. Therefore, females are competing indirectly for resources as opposed to males competing directly for mating opportunities.

Female Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys appear to substitute low-risk exchanges of dominance and submission for overt aggressive behavior, which is consistent with the aggressive-submissive hypothesis. It has been argued that if attacks between individuals are rare, a dominance hierarchy does not exist or is implicit (Isbell and Young, 2002; Koenig, 2002). However, this study confirmed that relationships between adult females in this species are still hierarchical. Similar results have been found in studies of Pan troglodytes (Goodall, 1986; Nishida, 1979, 1996) and R. roxellana (He et al., 2013). The dominance hierarchy manifests after repeated asymmetric agonistic interactions between individuals (de Waal and Luttrell, 1989), which may be limited to certain contexts that depend on environmental conditions. For instance, the distribution and relative scarcity in space and time can affect individuals’ mutual tolerance. R. bieti mainly feed on low-energy lichens, which are evenly distributed and consistently available, for most of the year. Scarce, high-quality foods such as the fruit of Cornus capitata appear only at certain times of year, which could increase aggression and reduce tolerance during that season as individuals scramble to consume those foods.

High-ranking individuals have high grooming income

The balance of giving vs. receiving in bouts of allogrooming can reflect the dominance hierarchy. It has been argued that in some species, such as the capuchin monkey (Cebus apella), allogrooming is an expression of mutual attraction, and its frequency is unaffected by rank or kinship status (O’Brien, 1993). However, our findings for grooming income in R. bieti were similar to patterns observed in green monkeys (Chlorocebus sabaeus; Fairbanks, 1980), bonobos (Pan paniscus; Franz, 1999), and stump-tailed macaques (Macaca arctoides; de Waal and Luttrell, 1988). Allogrooming behavior has a social function, reducing tension and regulating hierarchical relationships between group members (Terry, 1970) such that higher-ranking individuals receive more grooming. Allogrooming may serve as a ritualized way to reduce conflict, as low-ranking individuals might groom higher-ranking individuals in exchange for tolerance and reduced aggression (Terry, 1970). In wild R. roxellana, for instance, individuals that are attacked or threatened will groom the aggressor after the conflict has subsided (Ren et al., 1990). Adult female collared mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus lunulatus) that are attacked by another group member will attempt reconciliation via grooming to avoid being attacked again (Pérez and Veà, 1998), and in bonnet macaques (M. radiata), low-ranking individuals are not attacked by other group members while grooming high-ranking individuals (Joan, 1982). In short, a grooming economy can emerge when low-ranking individuals trade grooming for reduced conflict and protection (Sade, 1973; Gust and Gordon, 1993), allowing high-ranking individuals to gain more grooming income.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we presented the first detailed analysis of dominance hierarchies in wild adult females within the same OMU in R. bieti. We argue that these females do have hierarchical relationships, but that they substitute regular displays of aggression and submission for costly and potentially dangerous direct conflicts, consistent with the aggressive-submissive hypothesis. Similar to other primates, allogrooming behavior in R. bieti functions to reduce tension and reinforce the dominance hierarchy, allowing high-ranking individuals to benefit more from grooming. Grooming can also be used as a means of quelling conflicts or mitigating attacks, consistent with the resource control hypothesis.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to our field assistants Jianhua Yu, Jinming Yu and Lizhong Yu. We thank Baimaxueshan National Nature Reserve for our work permit. This research was funded by the National Key Program of Research and Development, Ministry of Science and Technology (No. 2017YFC0505205; 2016YFC0503200), National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31470461), Applied Basic Research Program of Sichuan Province (No. 2017JY0325), Project Supported by the Biodiversity Investigation, Observation and Assessment Program of Ministry of Ecology and Environment of China.

Statement of conflict of interest

Authors have declared no conflict of interest.

References

Campbell, C.J., Fuentes, A. and MacKinnon, K.C., 2006. Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, New York.

Claud, A.B., 1970. Coalitions among gelada baboons. Primates, 11: 327-333. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01730637

Coelho, A.M., Turner, S.A. and Bramblett, C.A., 1983. Allogrooming and social status: An assessment of the contributions of female behavior to the social organization of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas). Primates, 24: 184-197. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381081

Cui, L.W., Sun, Q.L. and Li, B.G., 2014. Dominance hierarchy and social relationships in a group of captive black-and-white snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti). Zool. Res., 35: 204-213.

de Waal, F.B.M. and Luttrell, L.M., 1988. Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relation characteristics or cognition. Ethol. Sociobiol., 8: 101-118. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(88)90016-7

de Waal, F.B.M. and Luttrell, L.M., 1989. Toward a comparative socioecology of the genus Macaca: Different dominance styles in rhesus and stumptail monkeys. Am. J. Primatol., 19: 83-109. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350190203

de Vries, H., Stevens, J.M.G. and Vervaecke, H., 2006, Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies. Anim. Behav., 71: 585-592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.05.015

Desrochers, A., 1989. Sex, dominance, and microhabitat use in wintering black-capped chickadees: A field experiment. Ecology, 70: 636-645. https://doi.org/10.2307/1940215

Emlen, S.T., 1982a. The evolution of helping. I. An ecological constraints model. Am. Natural., 119: 29-39. https://doi.org/10.1086/283888

Emlen, S.T., 1982b. The evolution of helping. II. The role of behavioral conflict. Am. Natural., 119: 40-53. https://doi.org/10.1086/283889

Emlen, S.T., 1994. Benefits, constraints and the evolution of the family. Trends Ecol. Evol., 9: 282-285. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(94)90030-2

Fairbanks, L.A., 1980. Relationships among adult females in captive vervet monkeys: Testing a model of rank-elated attractiveness. Anim. Behav., 28: 853-859. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80145-X

Franz, C., 1999. Allogrooming behavior and grooming site preferences in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): Association with female dominance. Int. J. Primatol., 20: 525-543.

Gammell, M.P., Vries, H. and Jennings, D.J., 2003. David’s score: A more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock et al. index. Anim. Behav., 66: 601-605. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2226

Goodall, J., 1986. The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, Harvard University Press, MA.

Gust, D.A. and Gordon, T.P., 1993. Conflict resolution in sooty mangabeys. Anim. Behav., 46: 685-694. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1245

He, H.X., Zhao, H.T. and Qi, X.G., 2013. Dominance rank of adult females and mating competition in Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains, China. Chin. Sci. Bull., 58: 2205-2211. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11434-012-5649-2

Isbell, L.A. and Young, T.P., 2002. Ecological models of female social relationships in primates: Similarities, disparities and some directions for future clarity. Behaviour, 139: 177-202. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853902760102645

Joan, B.S., 1982. Altruism among female Macaca radiata: Explanations and analysis of patterns of grooming and coalition formation. Behaviour, 79: 162-188. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853982X00238

Koenig, A., 2002. Competition for resources and its behavioral consequences among female primates. Int. J. Primatol., 23: 759-783. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015524931226

Li, B.G., Li, H.Q., Zhao, D.P., Zhang, Y.H. and Qi, X.G., 2006. Study on dominance hierarchy of the Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in Qinling Mountains. Acta Theriol. Sin., 26: 18-25.

Li, D.Y., Ren, B.P., Li, B.G. and Li, M., 2010. Range expansion as a response to increasing group size in the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey. Folia Primatol., 81: 315-329. https://doi.org/10.1159/000322515

Li, D.Y., Grueter, C.C., Ren, B.P., Li, M., Peng, Z.S. and Wei, F.W., 2013. Distribution of sleeping sites of the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus bieti) in the Samage Forest, China. Integr. Zool., 8: 327-334. https://doi.org/10.1111/1749-4877.12021

Li, Y.H., Li, D.Y., Ren, B.P., Hu, J., Li, B.G., Krzton, A. and Li, M., 2014. Differences in the Activity budgets of Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti) by age-sex class at Xiangguqing in Baimaxueshan Nature Reserve, China. Folia Primatol., 85: 335-342. https://doi.org/10.1159/000368831

Leinfelder, I., Vries, H.D. and Deleu, R., 2001. Rank and grooming reciprocity among females in mixed-sex group captive Hamadryas baboon. Am. J. Primatol., 55: 25-42. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1036

Melnick, D.J. and Pearl, M.C., 1987. Cercopithecines in multi-male groups: Genetic diversity and population structure. In: Primate societies (eds. B.B. Smuts, D.L. Cheney and R.M. Seyfarth). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 123-134.

Martin, P. and Bateson, P., 1993. Measuring behavior: An introductory guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 50-55. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139168342

Noe, R., Dewaal, F.E.M. and van Hooff, J.A.R.M., 1980. Types of dominance in a chimpanzee colony. Folia Primatol., 34: 90-110. https://doi.org/10.1159/000155949

Nishida, T. and Hosaka, K., 1996. Coalition strategies among adult male chimpanzees of Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. In: Great ape societies (eds. W.C. McGrew, L.F. Marchant and T. Nishida). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 114-134. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511752414.011

Nishida, T., 1979. The social structure of chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. In: The great apes (eds. D.A. Hamburg and E.R. McCown). Menlo Park, Benjamin/Cummings, CA, pp. 73-122.

Nishida, T., 1988. Development of social grooming between mother and offspring in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol., 50: 109-123. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156335

O’Brien, T.G., 1993. Allogrooming behavior among female wedge-capped capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav., 46: 499-510. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1218

Pérez, P.A. and Veà, J.J.,1998. Cost-benefit analysis of allogrooming behavior in primates. Primates, 50: 15-37.

Reeve, H.K. and Keller, L., 1995. Partitioning of reproduction in mother–daughter versus sibling associations: A test of optimal skew theory. Am. Natural., 145: 119-132. https://doi.org/10.1086/285731

Ren, R.M., Qi, H. and Liang, B., 1991. The reconciliation behavior of golden monkeys Rhinopithecus roxellana in small breeding groups. Primates, 32: 321-327. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02382673

Ren, R.M., Su, Y.J., Yan, K.H., Qi, H.J. and Bao, W.Y., 1990. Social relationships among golden monkeys in breeding cages. Acta Psychol. Sin., 22: 277-282.

Robert, M.S., 2005. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308: 648-652. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106477

Sade, D.S., 1973. An ethogram for rhesus monkeys: Antithetical contrasts in posture and movement. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol., 38: 537-542. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330380263

Terry, R.L., 1970. Primate grooming as a tension reduction mechanism. J. Psych., 76: 129-136. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1970.9916830

Vehrencamp, S.L., 1983. A model for the evolution of ‘despotic’ versus ‘egalitarian’ species. Anim. Behav., 31: 667-682. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80222-X

Walters, J.R. and Seyfarth, R.M., 1987. Conflict and cooperation. In: Primate societies (eds. B.B. Smuts, D.L. Cheney and R.M. Seyfarth). University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 306-317.

Wang, X.W., Li, B.G., Wu, X.M., He, P.J. and Hu, Y.L., 2007. Dominance hierarchy of Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) OMUs in Qinling Mountains by feeding superiority. Acta Theriol. Sin., 27: 344-349.

Wrangham, R.W., 1981. Drinking competition in vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav., 29: 904-910. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80027-9

Xia, W.C., Zhang, C., Zhuang, H.F., Ren, B.P., Zhou, J. and Shen, J., 2020. The potential distribution and disappearing of Yunnan snub-nosed monkey: influences of habitat fragmentation. Glob. Ecol. Conserv., 21: e00835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gecco.2019.e00835

Yu, P.L., Yang, J.Y., Bao, W.D., Yu, H.L., Yao, H. and Wu, F., 2009. Aggressive behaviors and the rank order of a provisioned group of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in Shennongjia. Acta Theriol. Sin., 29: 7-11.

Zhang, P., Watanabe, K. and Li, B.G., 2008. Female social dynamics in a provisioned free-ranging band of the Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains, China. Am. J. Primatol., 70: 1013-1022. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20591

Zhang, D.J., Li, D.Y., Hu, J., Ren, B.P., Yuan, X.X., He, X.M., Li, Y.H. and Li, M., 2014. Allogrooming among female Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti). Acta Theriol. Sin., 34: 38-45.

To share on other social networks, click on any share button. What are these?

Pakistan Journal of Zoology

December

Pakistan J. Zool., Vol. 56, Iss. 6, pp. 2501-3000

Featuring

Click here for more

Subscribe Today

Receive free updates on new articles, opportunities and benefits


Subscribe Unsubscribe