Submit or Track your Manuscript LOG-IN

Individual Identification Based on the Songs of the Emei Shan Liocichla (Liocichla omeiensis)

PJZ_52_2_617-624

 

 

Individual Identification Based on the Songs of the Emei Shan Liocichla (Liocichla omeiensis)

Chiping Kong1,2, Yongheng Wu1,2, Shangling Lou1, Benping Chen3, Simon D. Dowell4 and Yiqiang Fu1*

1Sichuan Institute Key Laboratory for Protecting Endangered Birds in the Southwest Mountains, College of Life Science, Leshan Normal University, Leshan 614004, Sichuan Province, China

2College of Animal Science, Xinjiang Agricultural University, Urumqi, 830052, Xinjiang Province, China

3Laojunshan National Nature Reserve Administration, Pingshan 645350, Sichuan Province, China

4North of England Zoological Society, Chester Zoo, Upton by Chester, Chester CH1 1LH, UK

ABSTRACT

In some oscine birds, individually distinctive vocalisations are thought to be more pronounced allowing individual recognition based on songs. In this study, we explore individual identification in the songs of male Emei Shan Liocichla, a vulnerable babbler that inhabits dense vegetation. We recorded territorial songs of male Emei Shan Liocichla from Laojunshan National Nature Reserve during the breeding season of 2016, in southern Sichuan, China. We used three methods to compare the acoustic difference among and within individuals. The results demonstrated that male Emei Shan Liocichlas had individually distinct songs and can be used to identify individuals. We also found that 4 to 5 songs were relatively certain of an individual identity. Our findings indicated that individual distinctiveness by vocalisation was reliable in the Emei Shan Liocichla. Monitoring song therefore provides a potential method to follow individuals in the field and thus facilitate ecological research of this secretive species.


Article Information

Received 10 May 2017

Revised 20 July 2017

Accepted 03 May 2019

Available online 24 January 2020

Authors’ Contribution

YF and CK designed the study. CK, YW and BC performed the experiments. CK, YW, SL, YF and SD carried out the analyses and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Key words

The Emei Shan Liocichla, Songs, Song features, Individual identification, Southern Sichuan

DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.17582/journal.pjz/20170510120508

* Corresponding author: fyq512@126.com

0030-9923/2020/0002-0617 $ 9.00/0

Copyright 2020 Zoological Society of Pakistan



INTRODUCTION

Many birds broadcast loud, easily detected sounds, especially during the breeding season. If these were individually distinctive, they could enable determination of the signalers’ identity according to their vocalisations, allowing other individuals to discriminate between the vocalisations of breeding mates, familiar neighbors or intruders (Tibbetts and Dale, 2007; Wilson et al., 2010; Stoddard et al., 1991; Aubin and Jouventin, 1998). “Neighbor–stranger discrimination” is contingent on the ability of birds to distinguish from among individuals and vocalisations provide an accessible source of variation upon which to base these distinctions (Stoddard, 1996; Lovell and Lein, 2004; Budka and Osiejuk, 2013).

The prerequisite for individual recognition is that vocalisations have characteristics that are unique to each individual (Dale et al., 2001; Tibbetts and Dale, 2007). Fine structural features of vocalisations might be beneficial to individual distinctiveness. Individual distinctness of bird’s vocalisations not only plays an important role in avian life history, but can also be used to monitor and manage bird populations. Identification of individuals is essential for research attempting to address a wide range of questions in bird biology. Traditional techniques such as radio-tracking and bird-banding involving artificial physical markers have been used to identify individuals (Grava et al., 2008; Arshad and Zakaria, 2011; Xia et al., 2012). However, capturing and marking birds may interfere with the behaviors of birds and compromise their welfare (Dixon, 2011; Rogers and Paton, 2005). In addition, many threatened species requiring detailed studies to assist with their conservation live in environments such as dense forests where detection and capture for tagging is difficult (Mennill and Vehrencamp, 2011) whilst others are mainly active at night, making direct observation difficult (Farnsworth and Russell, 2007; Odom and Mennill, 2010). Acoustic identification of individuals is a validated approach to solve these problems. Provided that a bird’s vocalisations were individually distinctive, they can be used to identify individuals. Furthermore, it is non-invasive, thereby minimizing the influence on bird’s behavior or movements (Mennill, 2011). Individual identification based on vocalisations has been used to estimate territory and home-range size (Betts et al., 2005), explore dispersal patterns (Laiolo et al., 2007) and forecast population size and dynamics (Hartwig, 2005).

The Emei Shan Liocichla (Liocichla omeiensis) is a small passerine and is endemic to a restricted range in southwestern China. The males sing characteristic songs (a loud and complex whistle) during the breeding season to maintain territories (Mackinnon et al., 2000; Fu et al., 2011; Fu et al., 2013). This species is principally distributed in montane forest between 1,000 and 2,400 m in south-central Sichuan and northeast Yunnan and frequents thick bamboo and deep scrub, making individuals difficult to observe in the field (Fu et al., 2011). Due to a declining population, narrow distribution range and severely fragmented habitat, the Emei Sahn Liocichla is regarded as a globally vulnerable species by the IUCN (IUCN, 2015). There are very no published studies documenting Emei Shan Liocichla vocalisations. Consequently, further investigation of the vocalisations of the Emei Shan Liocichla is imperative.

In this study, we aim to describe song variation in the Emei Shan Liocichla, and seek to document the potential for individual identity based on vocalisations.

 

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study area

Territorial songs of the Emei Shan Liocichla were recorded in Laojunshan National Nature Reserve (28° 39’–28° 43’ N, 103° 57’–104° 04’ E) (Fig. 1). From May to June 2016 we collected territorial songs from a total of 17 male Emei Shan Liocichlas. The study site was in southern Sichuan, China, with a subtropical climate, and annual average temperature from approximately 10 to 15°C and mean annual precipitation in excess of 1,500 mm annually. The vegetation at these sites is dominated by evergreen broad leaf forest and interspersed with some non-native coniferous forests and tea plantations (Fu et al., 2011).

Field data collection

The songs were recorded using a SONY ICD-SX 1000 digital voice recorder (Sony, China) and RODE NTG-2 external microphone (Rode, Australia). We kept the distance between microphone and the singing male liocichlas within 30m to obtain clear recordings. In order to ensure that recordings were appropriately assigned to the right individuals, we only recorded those songs that we were certain of belonging to different individuals according to their territories. In total, we recorded 315 songs from 17 males, ranging from 8 to 33 songs for each male.


 

Song classification and feature analysis

We used Goldwave 5.25 (Goldwave, Canada) to remove noise below 1 kHz, which is below the minimum frequency of the Emei Shan Liocichla vocalisations. We used Avisoft-SAS Lab Lite (Avisoft Bioacoustics, Germany) to analyze the songs and prepare sonograms. The sonogram parameters setting were indicated as follows: sampling frequency, 22.05 kHz; fast Fourier transform length, 256 points; hamming window: a frame size of 100% and an overlap of 50%, sample size, 16 bits, frequency resolution 86 Hz, and time resolution 5.8 msec (Lindholm and Lindén, 2007).

We classified song types according to their shapes on the spectrogram, mainly compared the number and sequence of different types of elements (Franco and Slabbekoorn, 2009). Song types that share most of the features (more than 75 percent) mentioned above were classified as the same song types (Baker et al., 2000). Songs that varied only in the number of added or omitted or repeats elements but were otherwise similar in their shapes on sonogram, we also classified as the same song type (Sandoval et al., 2014). We measured the repertoire size for the 17 males that we had recorded 20 or more songs (Sandoval et al., 2014). Only 11 of 17 males met this criterion.

For each song, we obtained frequency and temporal measurements, and we measured 28 variables in total, the 28 measured variables showed in Table I (we used the codes to instead the variables in the following), and sound spectrograms of part of the variables depicted in Fig. 2.

Statistical analyses

Firstly, we randomly selected 10 sequential songs from each individual in order to avoid bias from those individuals where we recorded more songs. We only

 

Table I. The measured variables of the Emei Shan Liocichla songs.

Variables

Code

Highest frequency of the song

FH

Lowest frequency of the song

FL

Frequency at the start of the song

FS

Frequency at the end of the song

FE

Duration of the song

DT

Highest frequency of the1st note

F1H

Lowest frequency of the 1st note

F1L

Frequency at the start of the 1st note

F1S

Frequency at the end of 1st note

F1E

Duration of the 1st note

T1

Duration between 1st note and 2nd note

T1-2

Highest frequency of the2nd note

F2H

Lowest frequency of the 2ndnote

F2L

Frequency at the start of the 2nd note

F2S

Frequency at the end of 2nd note

F2E

Duration of the 2nd note

T2

Duration between 2nd note and 3rd note

T2-3

Highest frequency of the3rd note

F3H

Lowest frequency of the 3rd note

F3L

Frequency at the start of the 3rd note

F3S

Frequency at the end of 3rd note

F3E

Duration of the 3rd note

T3

Duration between 3rd note and 4th note

T3-4

Highest frequency of the 4th note

F4H

Lowest frequency of the 4th note

F4L

Frequency at the start of the 4th note

F4S

Frequency at the end of 4th note

F4E

Duration of the 4th note

T4

Total number of elements of the song

N

 

recorded 8 songs (<10 songs) for two males, so all songs were used into subsequent analyses. We calculated mean values of measured temporal and frequency variables of each individual. Then, we used the data to calculate the overall mean for each variable. For each individual, we used coefficients of variation (CV) to describe the extent of variability for each measured variables (Charrier et al., 2001; Xia et al., 2014). We calculated between-male coefficients of variation among individuals (CVa) and coefficients of variation within individuals (CVw). Subsequently, we calculated the potential of individual coding (PIC) for each variable (Charrier et al., 2001; Xia et al., 2014). PIC value is equal to CVa /mean CVw, and mean CVw represented the average value of the CVw of all individuals. For each variable, PIC value exceeds 1 indicating that this variable is available for individual recognition. We used Wilcoxon test to compare the variation coefficient between and within individuals as the data were not distributed normally.


 

We complemented the potential of individual coding approach by utilizing discriminant function analysis (DFA), aimed at comparing differences in the measured temporal and frequency variables between males. We aimed to explore the fewest possible measured variables to explain the largest possible amount of differentiation between individuals. We established a model that included all measurements; we excluded from the discriminant analysis the variables with the lowest F-to-remove value until we obtained a model with the fewest variables that still provided the same or higher percentage of correct assignments relative to the original model that included all acoustic features. This analysis was conducted for each song type that was shared by more than 5 individuals and we had recorded at least 5 songs by each male; two song types satisfied these standards (as shown in Fig. 3). The results were conservative estimates and reported as percentages of classification accuracy based on the leave-one-out approach to cross-validation (Sandoval et al., 2014).

We also used discriminant function analyses to determine how much recording would have to record to be relatively certain of an individual identity. We mainly focused on the two dominant song types since we had recorded at least 5 songs for each individual. Firstly, we randomly selected 2 songs from each individual to conduct DFA. Then, we added the sample size of songs one by one until we obtained a stable high correct recognition rate. Our purpose was using the fewest sample size of songs while we were able to acquire stable high classification accuracy.

We used Kruskal-Wallis test to explore whether measured variables varied among different individuals since the data were not distributed normally. We only focused on the song types and variables that were detected by the discriminant function analysis as being favorable to individual identification.

All data are presented as means ± SE using two-tailed tests. Significant differences are determined at P = 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed with software SPSS 22.0 for Windows.

 

Table II. The features and coefficients of variation between and within individuals based on the measured variables of male Emei Shan Liocichla.

Variable

Mean ± SE

CVa (%)

Mean CVw (%)

PIC value

FH

3.86±0.37

9.59

6.55

1.47

FL

1.72±0.21

12.34

12.17

1.01

FS

2.89±0.44

15.14

10.31

1.47

FE

2.04±0.23

11.65

13.07

0.89

DT

2.18±0.25

11.41

14.86

0.77

F1H

3.75±0.37

10.09

6.82

1.48

F1L

2.43±0.17

6.96

9.90

0.70

F1S

2.81±0.43

15.18

13.07

1.16

F1E

2.90±0.18

6.27

7.54

0.83

T1

0.55±0.18

33.12

19.12

1.73

T1-2

0.14±0.03

18.88

19.52

0.97

F2H

3.56±0.52

14.50

8.06

1.80

F2L

2.16±0.34

15.68

12.09

1.30

F2S

3.06±0.58

19.06

25.29

0.75

F2E

2.60±0.40

15.49

10.96

1.41

T2

0.62±0.06

10.36

10.01

1.04

T2-3

0.12±0.03

25.73

23.84

1.08

F3H

3.18±0.20

6.15

5.83

1.06

F3L

1.96±0.34

17.42

13.12

1.33

F3S

2.70±0.41

15.15

12.95

1.17

F3E

2.40±0.44

18.39

17.05

1.08

T3

0.54±0.10

18.73

16.67

1.12

T3-4

0.09±0.04

39.90

24.66

1.62

F4H

3.02±0.22

7.35

3.85

1.91

F4L

1.80±0.925

16.19

7.50

2.16

F4S

2.39±0.29

12.37

9.43

1.31

F4E

1.99±0.30

14.73

7.52

1.96

T4

0.63±0.06

9.21

8.88

1.04

P < 0.01, Z=2.983, Wilcoxon test between CVa and mean CVw.


 

RESULTS

Song features

We found 9 unique song types of 17 male Emei Shan Liocichlas from Laojunshan National Nature Reserve, and two song types were dominant in this population, which were respectively shared by 10 and 13 males. The repertoire sizes varied from 2 to 4, mean 2.55±0.69 for the 11 males with more than 20 songs. A complete song usually consists of two to four elements, mean 3.07±0.61 for the 17 males, more detail of the measured song variables see Table II. We also detected that the Emei Shan Liocichla prefer to generate various kinds of song type variants, mainly through the following method: omit, add or repeat an element of a whole song.

 

Table III. Results of discriminant function analysis used to evaluate individual identification in the Emei Shan Liocichla songs.

Song type

N

Percent correct classification (%)

Wilks’λ

Fdf

Variables retained in backwards DFA

Type I

10

97.4

<0.001

49116

F1H, F2H, T1, T1-2, T2-3, FS

Type II

13

96.3

<0.001

84314

DT, F1L, F1E, F2L, F2E, F3H, F3S, T1-2, T3, FH, FL, FE

 

Sample size (N) shows the total number of males that sang each song type. The Wilks’λ and F values show the results of backwards DFA. The features retained in the backwards DFA correspond to the measured temporal and frequency variables.

Coefficients of variation

PIC values were greater than 1.0 for most of the measured variables (22 of 28 variables exceeded 1).There were significant variant between CVa and mean CVw by Wilcoxon test (P < 0.01, Z=2.983, as shown in Table II), suggesting that variation between individuals was greater than within individuals.

Discriminant analyses

Discriminant function analyses on the basis of the two dominant song types allocated songs to the correct male and produced a high percentage of correct classification, ranging from 96.3% to 97.4%, indicating a high rate of individual identification. This high rate of discrimination was acquired with a segment of measured variables retained in backwards DFA, six typical variables for song type I, and twelve typical variables for song type II, as shown in Table III.


 

The result of DFA also showed that the classification accuracy rose with the increasing sample size of songs for the two dominant song types. To get a reliable identity on an individual bird (i.e. the correct rates were nearly 90%), 4 to 5 songs are needed at least (Fig. 4).

 

Table IV. Results of Kruskal-Wallis test used to compare typical variables among different individuals.

Song type

Typical variables

χ2

df

P Value

Type I

F1H

68.084

9

<0.01

F2H

70.218

9

<0.01

T1

71.259

9

<0.01

T1-2

41.091

9

<0.01

T2-3

46.854

9

<0.01

FS

49.061

9

<0.01

Type II

DT

69.958

12

<0.01

F1L

75.676

12

<0.01

F1E

54.245

12

<0.01

F2L

91.221

12

<0.01

F2E

88.742

12

<0.01

F3H

69.636

12

<0.01

F3S

84.504

12

<0.01

T1-2

47.925

12

<0.01

T3

77.925

12

<0.01

FH

87.468

12

<0.01

FL

81.384

12

<0.01

FE

70.212

12

<0.01

Typical variables were retained in backwards DFA from two dominant song types.

 

Kruskal-Wallis test

We analyzed the variance among individuals of typical variables of two dominant song types that were detected by the discriminant function analysis. The results of Kruskal-Wallis test indicated that the typical variables for the two dominant song types that we analyzed were significant variant among different individuals (song type I: df=9, P<0.01; song type II: df=12, P<0.01; as shown in Table IV).

 

DISCUSSION

This study demonstrated that male Emei Shan Liocichlas had individually distinct songs. The potential of individual coding (PIC) revealed that the variation of measured song variables among individuals were greater than within individuals. DFA based on the two dominant song types yielded a high level of accuracy. And Kruskal-Wallis test indicated that the typical variables were significant variant among different individuals. may among, Our findings showed that the individual distinctiveness by vocalisation was reliable in the Emei Shan Liocichla, which provides a potential method to follow individuals in the field and thus facilitate ecological research (such as population density monitoring and breeding behavior studies) of this secretive species.

Discriminant function analyses also acquired high correct recognition rates only based on 4 to 5 songs for each individual. This would be a useful analysis as it would inform ecologists working on this species that only 4 to 5 recordings they would have to collect to be relatively certain of an individual identity.

Steere’s liocichla (Liocichla steerii), a close relative of Emei Shan Liocichla, exhibits a characteristic duetting behavior, which is thought to function as a joint-territory defense (Yuan et al., 2005). The leading male song in duets of Steere’s liocichlas was highly individualistic (Weng et al., 2012; Mays et al., 2006). That would create selection for individuality so that females may answer the song of their mates (Mays et al., 2006). However, we didn’t find this behavior in Emei Shan Liocichla, the female Emei Shan Liocichlas are incapable to sing a song (Kong, C.P., unpublished data). How this difference evolved between the two species is a problem needs further study.

In bird species, individually distinctive vocalisations have been reported not only in oscine birds (Nelson and Poesel, 2007; Ellis, 2008; Benedict and McEntee, 2009), but also in a number of sub-oscine and non-oscine bird species (Fitzsimmons et al., 2008; Sandoval and Escalante, 2011; Garcia et al., 2012). Consequently, we speculate that avian individual identification by vocalisations might be a common phenomenon in nature. Further work is needed to collect more examples in wider bird species and ascertain its evolutionary mechanism. In addition, we suggest that more attention should be paid to whether the song features of birds are consistent over time as it is imperative to identify individuals steadily.

 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31272330), the National Forestry and Grassland Administration of China and the Scientific Research Innovation Team Projects of Leshan Normal University. We thank the Laojunshan National Nature Reserve for allowing us to conduct this study. We are grateful to Ming Xiang for help with the fieldwork, and Canwei Xia for suggestions on the paper. We also greatly appreciate the comments and suggestions by two anonymous reviewers.

 

Statement of conflict of interest

The authors have declared that no competing interests.

 

REFERENCE

Arshad, M.I. and Zakaria, M., 2011. Variation in home range size exhibited by Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus) in oil palm plantation habitat, Malaysia. Pakistan J. Zool., 43: 833-840.

Aubin, T. and Jouventin, P., 1998. Cocktail-party effect in king penguin colonies. Proc. R.S. B Biol. Sci., 265: 1665. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0486

Baker, M.C., Howard, T.M. and Sweet, P.W., 2000. Microgeographic variation and sharing of the gargle vocalization and its component syllables in black-capped chickadee (Aves, Paridae, Poecile Atricapillus) populations. Ethology, 106: 819–838. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00602.x

Benedict, L. and Mcentee, J.P., 2009. Context, structural variability and distinctiveness of California Towhee (Pipilo crissalis) vocal duets. Ethology, 115: 77–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01583.x

Betts, M.G., Hadley, A.S. and Doran, P.J., 2005. Avian mobbing response is restricted by territory boundaries: experimental evidence from two species of forest warblers. Ethology, 111: 821–835. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01109.x

Budka, M. and Osiejuk, T.S., 2013. Neighbour–stranger call discrimination in a nocturnal rail species, the Corncrake Crex crex. J. Ornithol., 154: 685-694. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-013-0933-8

Catchpole, C.K. and Slater, P.J.B., 2008. Bird song: Biological themes and variations. 2nd ed. Cambridge University Press, New York. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511754791

Charrier, I., Jouventin, P., Mathevon, N. and Aubin, T., 2001. Individual identity coding depends on call type in the south polar skua Catharacta maccormicki. Polar Biol., 24: 378-382. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003000100231

Dale, J., Lank, D.B. and Reeve, H.K., 2001. Signaling individual identity versus quality: A model and case studies with ruffs, queleas and house finches. Am. Natural., 158: 75-86. https://doi.org/10.1086/320861

Dixon, A., 2011. Effects of attaching telemetry equipment to free-living birds. Vet. Rec., 169: 308-309. https://doi.org/10.1136/vr.d5778

Ellis, J.M.S., 2008. Decay of apparent individual distinctiveness in the begging calls of adult female white-throated magpie-jays. Condor, 110: 648-657. https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2008.8592

Farnsworth, A. and Russell, R.W., 2007. Monitoring flight calls of migrating birds from an oil platform in the northern gulf of mexico. J. Field Ornithol., 78: 279–289. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1557-9263.2007.00115.x

Fitzsimmons, L.P., Barker, N.K. and Mennill, D.J., 2008. Individual variation and lek-based vocal distinctiveness in songs of the screaming piha (Lipaugus vociferans), a suboscine songbird. The Auk, 125: 908-914. https://doi.org/10.1525/auk.2008.07128

Franco, P. and Slabbekoorn, H., 2009. Repertoire size and composition in great tits: a flexibility test using playbacks. Anim. Behav., 77: 261-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.09.023

Fu, Y., Dowell, S.D. and Zhang, Z., 2011. Breeding ecology of the Emei Shan Liocichla (Liocichla omeiensis). Wilson J. Ornithol., 123: 748-754. https://doi.org/10.1676/11-008.1

Fu, Y., Dowell, S.D. and Zhang, Z., 2013. The Emei Shan Liocichla: population, behavior and conservation. Chinese Birds, 4: 260-264. https://doi.org/10.5122/cbirds.2013.0023

Garcia, M., Charrier, I., Rendall, D. and Iwaniuk, A.N., 2012. Temporal and spectral analyses reveal individual variation in a non-vocal acoustic display: the drumming display of the ruffed grouse (Bonasa umbellus, L.). Ethology, 118: 292-301. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02011.x

Grava, T., Mathevon, N., Place, E. and Balluet, P., 2008. Individual acoustic monitoring of the European eagle owl Bubo bubo. Ibis, 150: 279–287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919X.2007.00776.x

Hartwig, S., 2005. Individual acoustic identification as a non-invasive conservation tool: an approach to the conservation of the African wild dog Lycaon pictus (Temminck, 1820). Bioacoustics, 15: 35-50. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2005.9753537

IUCN, 2015. The IUCN red list of threatened species. Version 2015-4. Available from: http://www.iucnredlist.org (accessed 5 March 2016).

Laiolo, P., Vögeli, M., Serrano, D. and Tella, J.L., 2007. Testing acoustic versus physical marking: two complementary methods for individual-based monitoring of elusive species. J. Avian Biol., 38: 1-3. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2007.0908-8857.04006.x

Lindholm, A. and Lindén, A., 2007. Some notes on the distribution and songs of two Oriental Cuckoo taxa, Cuculus (saturatus) saturatus and Cuculus (saturatus) optatus. Forktail, 23: 12-16.

Lovell, S.F. and Lein, M.R., 2004. Neighbor-stranger discrimination by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher, Empidonax alnorum. Behav. Ecol., 15: 799-804. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh082

Mackinnon, J., Showler, D., Phillipps, K. and Mackinnon, J., 2000. A field guide to the birds of china: ornithology. Colonial Waterbirds, 18: 841-843.

Mays, H.L., Yao, C.T. and Yuan, H.W., 2006. Antiphonal duetting in Steere’s liocichla (Liocichla steerii): male song individuality and correlation between habitat and duetting behavior. Ecol. Res., 21: 311-314. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11284-005-0115-0

Mennill, D. J., 2011. Individual distinctiveness in avian vocalisations and the spatial monitoring of behaviour. Ibis, 153: 235–238. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919X.2011.01119.x

Mennill, D.J. and Vehrencamp, S.L., 2011. Context-dependent functions of avian duets revealed by microphone-array recordings and multispeaker playback. Curr. Biol. Cb, 18: 1314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.073

Nelson, D.A. and Poesel, A., 2007. Segregation of information in a complex acoustic signal: individual and dialect identity in white-crowned sparrow song. Anim. Behav., 74: 1073-1084. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.018

Odom, K.J. and Mennill, D.J., 2010. Vocal duets in a nonpasserine: an examination of territory defence and neighbour–stranger discrimination in a neighbourhood of barred owls. Behaviour, 147: 619-639. https://doi.org/10.1163/000579510X12632972452424

Rogers, D.J. and Paton, D.C., 2005. Acoustic identification of individual Rufous Bristle birds, a threatened species with complex song repertoires. Emu, 105: 203–210. https://doi.org/10.1071/MU05040

Sandoval, L., Méndez, C. and Mennill, D.J., 2014. Individual distinctiveness in the fine structural features and repertoire characteristics of the songs of white-eared ground-sparrows. Ethology, 120: 275–286. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12206

Sandoval, L. and Escalante, I., 2011. Song description and individual variation in males of the common pauraque (Nyctidromus albicollis). Ornitol. Neotrop., 22: 173-185.

Stoddard, P.K., Beecher, M.D., Horning, C.L. and Campbell, S.E., 1991. Recognition of individual neighbors by song in the song sparrow, a species with song repertoires. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 29: 211-215. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00166403

Stoddard, P.K., 1996. Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines. Cornell University Press. pp. 356-374.

Tibbetts, E.A. and Dale, J., 2007. Individual recognition: it is good to be different. Trends Ecol. Evolut., 22: 529-37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2007.09.001

Weng, Y.S., Yuan, H.W., Yao, C.T. and Hsieh, C.F., 2012. Male and female Steere’s liocichlas respond differently to solo and stereo duet playback. Anim. Behav., 83: 487-493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.024

Wilson, D.R. and Mennill, D.J., 2010. Black-capped chickadees (Poecilea tricapillus) use individually distinctive songs to discriminate between conspecifics. Anim. Behav., 79: 1267-1275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.02.028

Xia, C., Lin, X., Liu, W., Lloyd, H. and Zhang, Y., 2012. Acoustic identification of individuals within large avian populations: A case study of the brownish-flanked bush warbler, south-central China. PLoS One, 7: 1975-1982. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042528

Xia, C., Xiao, H. and Zhang, Y., 2014. Individual variation in brownish-flanked bush warbler songs. Condor, 112: 591-595. https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2010.090021

Yuan, H.W., Hsieh, C.F., Peng, Y.W., Yao, C.T. and Mays, H.L., 2005. Duetting behavior in Steere’s liocichla (Liocichla steerii): Does duet song function as a joint-territory defense? Animal Behavior Meeting, Snowbird, US.

To share on other social networks, click on any share button. What are these?

Pakistan Journal of Zoology

April

Pakistan J. Zool., Vol. 56, Iss. 2, pp. 503-1000

Featuring

Click here for more

Subscribe Today

Receive free updates on new articles, opportunities and benefits


Subscribe Unsubscribe